Child Care Center Curriculum Plan Involving Families to Enhance Social Emotional Behaviors

Foundations

  • Interactions with Adults
  • Relationships with Adults
  • Interactions with Peers
  • Relationships with Peers
  • Identity of Cocky in Relation to Others
  • Recognition of Power
  • Expression of Emotion
  • Empathy
  • Emotion Regulation
  • Impulse Control
  • Social Understanding

References

Return to Contents

Social-emotional development includes the child's experience, expression, and management of emotions and the power to constitute positive and rewarding relationships with others (Cohen and others 2005). It encompasses both intra- and interpersonal processes.

The core features of emotional development include the ability to identify and empathize one's own feelings, to accurately read and embrace emotional states in others, to manage strong emotions and their expression in a constructive fashion, to regulate 1's own behavior, to develop empathy for others, and to establish and maintain relationships. (National Scientific Council on the Developing Kid 2004, 2)

Infants experience, express, and perceive emotions before they fully understand them. In learning to recognize, characterization, manage, and communicate their emotions and to perceive and attempt to understand the emotions of others, children build skills that connect them with family, peers, teachers, and the community. These growing capacities help young children to get competent in negotiating increasingly complex social interactions, to participate effectively in relationships and group activities, and to reap the benefits of social back up crucial to healthy man development and performance.

Healthy social-emotional evolution for infants and toddlers unfolds in an interpersonal context, namely that of positive ongoing relationships with familiar, nurturing adults. Young children are specially attuned to social and emotional stimulation. Even newborns appear to attend more to stimuli that resemble faces (Johnson and others 1991). They as well prefer their mothers' voices to the voices of other women (DeCasper and Fifer 1980). Through nurturance, adults support the infants' earliest experiences of emotion regulation (Bronson 2000a; Thompson and Goodvin 2005).

Responsive caregiving supports infants in starting time to regulate their emotions and to develop a sense of predictability, condom, and responsiveness in their social environments. Early relationships are so important to developing infants that research experts accept broadly concluded that, in the early years, "nurturing, stable and consistent relationships are the central to healthy growth, development and learning" (National Enquiry Quango and Institute of Medicine 2000, 412). In other words, high-quality relationships increase the likelihood of positive outcomes for young children (Shonkoff 2004). Experiences with family members and teachers provide an opportunity for immature children to learn about social relationships and emotions through exploration and predictable interactions. Professionals working in child intendance settings tin can support the social-emotional development of infants and toddlers in various ways, including interacting straight with young children, communicating with families, arranging the physical space in the care environment, and planning and implementing curriculum.

Brain research indicates that emotion and cognition are profoundly interrelated processes. Specifically, "contempo cognitive neuroscience findings suggest that the neural mechanisms underlying emotion regulation may be the same as those underlying cognitive processes" (Bong and Wolfe 2004, 366). Emotion and cognition work together, jointly informing the child'due south impressions of situations and influencing behavior. Near learning in the early on years occurs in the context of emotional supports (National Enquiry Council and Institute of Medicine 2000). "The rich interpenetrations of emotions and cognitions establish the major psychic scripts for each child's life" (Panksepp 2001). Together, emotion and knowledge contribute to attentional processes, determination making, and learning (Cacioppo and Berntson 1999). Furthermore, cognitive processes, such as decision making, are affected past emotion (Barrett and others 2007). Brain structures involved in the neural circuitry of knowledge influence emotion and vice versa (Barrett and others 2007). Emotions and social behaviors bear on the young child'south ability to persist in goal-oriented activeness, to seek help when it is needed, and to participate in and benefit from relationships.

Young children who exhibit good for you social, emotional, and behavioral adjustment are more probable to have expert academic performance in elementary school (Cohen and others 2005; Cypher to Three 2004). The abrupt distinction between knowledge and emotion that has historically been made may exist more of an antiquity of scholarship than it is representative of the mode these processes occur in the brain (Barrett and others 2007). This recent enquiry strengthens the view that early childhood programs support later positive learning outcomes in all domains by maintaining a focus on the promotion of good for you social emotional development (National Scientific Council on the Developing Child 2004; Raver 2002; Shonkoff 2004).

Interactions with Adults

Interactions with adults are a frequent and regular part of infants' daily lives. Infants as immature as three months of historic period take been shown to exist able to discriminate between the faces of unfamiliar adults (Barrera and Maurer 1981). The foundations that depict Interactions with Adults and Relationships with Adults are interrelated. They jointly give a picture of good for you social-emotional evolution that is based in a supportive social surroundings established by adults. Children develop the ability to both respond to adults and engage with them first through predictable interactions in close relationships with parents or other caring adults at abode and exterior the home. Children use and build upon the skills learned through shut relationships to collaborate with less familiar adults in their lives. In interacting with adults, children engage in a broad variety of social exchanges such as establishing contact with a relative or engaging in storytelling with an infant intendance teacher.

Quality in early on childhood programs is, in large part, a function of the interactions that have place between the adults and children in those programs. These interactions class the basis for the relationships that are established between teachers and children in the classroom or home and are related to children's developmental condition. How teachers interact with children is at the very heart of early childhood education (Kontos and Wilcox-Herzog 1997, 11).

Foundation: Interactions with Adults

Return to Top

Relationships with Adults

Close relationships with adults who provide consistent nurturance strengthen children's capacity to learn and develop. Moreover, relationships with parents, other family unit members, caregivers, and teachers provide the key context for infants' social-emotional development. These special relationships influence the infant's emerging sense of self and understanding of others. Infants use relationships with adults in many means: for reassurance that they are safe, for assist in alleviating distress, for help with emotion regulation, and for social approving or encouragement. Establishing shut relationships with adults is related to children'southward emotional security, sense of self, and evolving agreement of the earth around them. Concepts from the literature on attachment may be applied to early childhood settings, in because the babe care instructor's role in separations and reunions during the day in care, facilitating the child's exploration, providing comfort, meeting physical needs, modeling positive relationships, and providing back up during stressful times (Raikes 1996).

Foundation: Relationships with Adults

Return to Top

Interactions with Peers

In early on infancy children interact with each other using simple behaviors such every bit looking at or touching some other child. Infants' social interactions with peers increase in complexity from engaging in repetitive or routine back-and-forth interactions with peers (for example, rolling a ball back and along) to engaging in cooperative activities such as building a tower of blocks together or acting out dissimilar roles during pretend play. Through interactions with peers, infants explore their involvement in others and larn virtually social behavior/social interaction. Interactions with peers provide the context for social learning and problem solving, including the experience of social exchanges, cooperation, turn-taking, and the sit-in of the showtime of empathy. Social interactions with peers likewise let older infants to experiment with dissimilar roles in small groups and in different situations such as relating to familiar versus unfamiliar children. As noted, the foundations called Interactions with Adults, Relationships with Adults, Interactions with Peers, and Relationships with Peers are interrelated. Interactions are stepping-stones to relationships. Burk (1996, 285) writes:

We, equally teachers, demand to facilitate the development of a psychologically prophylactic environment that promotes positive social interaction. Every bit children interact openly with their peers, they larn more near each other as individuals, and they begin building a history of interactions.

Foundation: Interaction with Peers

Return to Top

Relationships with Peers

Infants develop shut relationships with children they know over a period of fourth dimension, such as other children in the family child intendance setting or neighborhood. Relationships with peers provide immature children with the opportunity to develop strong social connections. Infants oftentimes prove a preference for playing and being with friends, every bit compared with peers with whom they do not have a relationship. Howes' (1983) research suggests that at that place are distinctive patterns of friendship for the infant, toddler, and preschooler age groups. The 3 groups vary in the number of friendships, the stability of friendships, and the nature of interaction between friends (for example, the extent to which they involve object exchange or exact communication).

Foundation: Relationships with Peers

Return to Superlative

Identity of Self in Relation to Others

Infants' social-emotional development includes an emerging sensation of self and others. Infants demonstrate this foundation in a number of ways. For instance, they tin can respond to their names, point to their body parts when asked, or name members of their families. Through an emerging understanding of other people in their social environment, children gain an understanding of their roles within their families and communities. They also become aware of their own preferences and characteristics and those of others.

Foundation: Identity of Cocky in Relation to Others

Return to Top

Recognition of Power

Infants' developing sense of self-efficacy includes an emerging understanding that they tin can make things happen and that they have item abilities. Self-efficacy is related to a sense of competency, which has been identified equally a basic human need (Connell 1990). The evolution of children's sense of self-efficacy may be seen in play or exploratory behaviors when they human activity on an object to produce a result. For example, they pat a musical toy to make sounds come up out. Older infants may demonstrate recognition of ability through "I" statements, such as "I did information technology" or "I'thou proficient at drawing."

Foundation: Recognition of Ability

Return to Summit

Expression of Emotion

Even early in infancy, children express their emotions through facial expressions, vocalizations, and body linguistic communication. The later ability to apply words to limited emotions gives young children a valuable tool in gaining the help or social back up of others (Saarni and others 2006). Temperament may play a role in children's expression of emotion. Tronick (1989, 112) described how expression of emotion is related to emotion regulation and communication between the mother and infant: "the emotional expressions of the baby and the caretaker function to allow them to mutually regulate their interactions . . . the babe and the adult are participants in an affective communication system."

Both the understanding and expression of emotion are influenced past culture. Cultural factors affect children'south growing agreement of the meaning of emotions, the developing knowledge of which situations atomic number 82 to which emotional outcomes, and their learning near which emotions are appropriate to display in which situations (Thompson and Goodvin 2005). Some cultural groups appear to express certain emotions more than ofttimes than other cultural groups (Tsai, Levenson, and McCoy 2006). In addition, cultural groups vary past which particular emotions or emotional states they value (Tsai, Knutson, and Fung 2006). Ane study suggests that cultural differences in exposure to particular emotions through storybooks may contribute to young children's preferences for detail emotional states (for example, excited or at-home) (Tsai and others 2007).

Immature children'due south expression of positive and negative emotions may play a significant part in their development of social relationships. Positive emotions entreatment to social partners and seem to enable relationships to form, while problematic management or expression of negative emotions leads to difficulty in social relationships (Denham and Weissberg 2004). The use of emotion-related words appears to be associated with how likable preschoolers are considered by their peers. Children who utilize emotion-related words were found to be ameliorate-liked by their classmates (Fabes and others 2001). Infants reply more positively to developed vocalizations that have a positive melancholia tone (Fernald 1993). Social smiling is a developmental process in which neurophysiology and cognitive, social, and emotional factors play a role, seen as a "reflection and constituent of an interactive relationship" (Messinger and Fogel 2007, 329). It appears likely that the feel of positive emotions is a particularly important contributor to emotional well-being and psychological health (Fredrickson 2000, 2003; Panksepp 2001).

Foundation: Expression of Emotion

Return to Top

Empathy

During the first 3 years of life, children begin to develop the capacity to feel the emotional or psychological state of some other person (Zahn-Waxler and Radke-Yarrow 1990). The following definitions of empathy are institute in the research literature: "knowing what another person is feeling," "feeling what another person is feeling," and "responding compassionately to some other'southward distress" (Levenson and Ruef 1992, 234). The concept of empathy reflects the social nature of emotion, as it links the feelings of ii or more than people (Levenson and Ruef 1992). Since human being life is relationship-based, ane vitally important role of empathy over the life span is to strengthen social bonds (Anderson and Keltner 2002). Research has shown a correlation between empathy and prosocial beliefs (Eisenberg 2000). In particular, prosocial behaviors, such as helping, sharing, and comforting or showing business for others, illustrate the development of empathy (Zahn-Waxler and others 1992) and how the feel of empathy is idea to exist related to the development of moral behavior (Eisenberg 2000). Adults model prosocial/empathic behaviors for infants in diverse ways. For example, those behaviors are modeled through caring interactions with others or through providing nurturance to the infant. Quann and Wien (2006, 28) suggest that one way to support the evolution of empathy in young children is to create a civilisation of caring in the early babyhood surroundings: "Helping children empathise the feelings of others is an integral aspect of the curriculum of living together. The relationships among teachers, between children and teachers, and among children are fostered with warm and caring interactions."

Foundation: Empathy

Return to Elevation

Emotion Regulation

The developing ability to regulate emotions has received increasing attention in the research literature (Eisenberg, Champion, and Ma 2004). Researchers have generated various definitions of emotion regulation, and debate continues as to the most useful and appropriate way to define this concept (Eisenberg and Spinrad 2004). Every bit a construct, emotion regulation reflects the interrelationship of emotions, cognitions, and behaviors (Bell and Wolfe 2004). Young children'due south increasing understanding and skill in the use of language is of vital importance in their emotional development, opening new avenues for communicating about and regulating emotions (Campos, Frankel, and Camras 2004) and helping children to negotiate acceptable outcomes to emotionally charged situations in more effective ways. Emotion regulation is influenced by culture and the historical era in which a person lives: cultural variability in regulation processes is pregnant (Mesquita and Frijda 1992). "Cultures vary in terms of what one is expected to feel, and when, where, and with whom ane may express dissimilar feelings" (Cheah and Rubin 2003, 3). Adults tin provide positive part models of emotion regulation through their behavior and through the verbal and emotional support they offer children in managing their emotions. Responsiveness to infants' signals contributes to the evolution of emotion regulation. Adults support infants' development of emotion regulation by minimizing exposure to excessive stress, chaotic environments, or over- or understimulation.

Emotion regulation skills are important in part because they play a role in how well children are liked past peers and teachers and how socially competent they are perceived to be (National Scientific Council on the Developing Child 2004). Children's power to regulate their emotions appropriately can contribute to perceptions of their overall social skills as well as to the extent to which they are liked past peers (Eisenberg and others 1993). Poor emotion regulation tin can impair children's thinking, thereby compromising their judgment and decision making (National Scientific Council on the Developing Child 2004). At kindergarten entry, children demonstrate broad variability in their ability to self-regulate (National Inquiry Council and Institute of Medicine 2000).

Foundation: Emotion Regulation

Return to Top

Impulse Command

Children's developing capacity to control impulses helps them adapt to social situations and follow rules. As infants grow, they get increasingly able to exercise voluntary control over behavior such as waiting for needs to be met, inhibiting potentially hurtful behavior, and interim according to social expectations, including safe rules. Grouping care settings provide many opportunities for children to do their impulse-command skills. Peer interactions often offer natural opportunities for young children to practice impulse command, equally they make progress in learning about cooperative play and sharing. Young children's agreement or lack of understanding of requests made of them may be one factor contributing to their responses (Kaler and Kopp 1990).

Foundation: Impulse Control

Return to Meridian

Social Understanding

During the infant/toddler years, children begin to develop an understanding of the responses, advice, emotional expression, and deportment of other people. This development includes infants' agreement of what to expect from others, how to engage in back-and-forth social interactions, and which social scripts are to be used for which social situations. "At each historic period, social cerebral understanding contributes to social competence, interpersonal sensitivity, and an awareness of how the self relates to other individuals and groups in a circuitous social world" (Thompson 2006, 26). Social agreement is especially of import because of the social nature of humans and human life, even in early on infancy (Wellman and Lagattuta 2000). Recent research suggests that infants' and toddlers' social understanding is related to how often they feel developed advice about the thoughts and emotions of others (Taumoepeau and Ruffman 2008).

Foundation: Social Understanding

Return to Top

References

Ainsworth, M. D. 1967. Infancy in Republic of uganda: Infant Intendance and the Growth of Love. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

American University of Pediatrics. 2004. Caring for Your Baby and Immature Child: Birth to Age 5 (Fourth edition). Edited past South. P. Shelov and R. E. Hannemann. New York: Bantam Books.

Anderson, C., and D. Keltner. 2002. "The Role of Empathy in the Formation and Maintenance of Social Bonds," Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Vol. 25, No. 1, 21–22.

Barrera, G. E., and D. Maurer. 1981. "Bigotry of Strangers past the 3-Calendar month-Old," Child Development, Vol. 52, No. 2, 558–63.

Barrett, 50., and others. 2007. "The Feel of Emotion," Annual Review of Psychology, Vol.58, 373–403.

Bates, E. 1990. "Language About Me and You: Pronomial Reference and the Emerging Concept of Self," in The Self in Transition: Infancy to Childhood. Edited by D. Cicchetti and Chiliad. Beeghly. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Bayley, Northward. 2006. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Third edition). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Cess, Inc.

Bell, One thousand., and C. Wolfe. 2004. "Emotion and Cognition: An Intricately Bound Developmental Process," Child Development, Vol. 75, No. two, 366–lxx.

Bergman, A., and A. Wilson. 1984. "Thoughts Near Stages on the Way to Empathy and the Capacity for Concern," in Empathy Ii. Edited by J. Lichtenberg, G. Bornstein, and D. Argent. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Bowlby, J. 1983. Attachment (2d edition), Zipper and Loss series, Vol. i. Foreword by Allan N. Schore. New York: Bones Books.

Brazelton, T. B. 1992. Touchpoints: Your Child's Emotional and Behavioral Development. New York: Perseus Publishing.

Bretherton, I., and others. 1986. "Learning to Talk About Emotions: A Functionalist Perspective," Child Development, Vol. 57, 529–48.

Bridges, L. J.; W. S. Grolnick; and J. P Connell. 1997. "Infant Emotion Regulation with Mothers and Fathers," Infant Behavior and Development, Vol. 20, 47–57.

Bronson, Thou. West. 1972. "Infants' Reaction to Unfamiliar Persons and Novel Objects," Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 37, No. 148, i–46.

Bronson, M. 2000a. "Recognizing and Supporting the Development of Self-Regulation in Young Children," Young Children, Vol. 55, No. 2, 32–37.

Bronson, M. 2000b. Cocky-Regulation in Early Childhood: Nature and Nurture. New York: Guilford Press.

Bullock, M., and P. Lutkenhaus. 1988. "The Development of Volitional Behavior in the Toddler Years," Kid Evolution, Vol. 59, 664–74.

Bullock, M., and P. Lutkenhaus. 1990. "Who Am I? Self-Understanding in Toddlers," Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Vol. 36, 217–38.

Burk, D. I. 1996. "Understanding Friendship and Social Interaction," Babyhood Pedagogy, Vol. 72, No. five, 282–85.

Cacioppo, J., and G. Berntson. 1999. "The Affect System: Architecture and Operating Characteristics," Current Directions in Psychological Science, Vol. eight, No. v, 133–37.

California Department of Didactics (CDE). 2005. "Desired Results Developmental Contour (DRDP)" (accessed February vii, 2007) Sacramento: California Department of Pedagogy.

Return to Peak

Campos, J.; C. Frankel; and Fifty. Camras. 2004. "On the Nature of Emotion Regulation," Child Development, Vol. 75, No. 2, 377–94.

Caufield, R. 1995. "Reciprocity Betwixt Infants and Caregivers During the First Yr of Life," Early Childhood Education Journal, Vol. 23, No. 1, iii–viii.

Cheah, C., and Yard. Rubin. 2003. "European American and Mainland Chinese Mothers' Socialization Beliefs Regarding Preschoolers' Social Skills," Parenting: Science and Practise, Vol. iii, No. 1, 1–21

Cohen, J., and others. 2005. Helping Immature Children Succeed: Strategies to Promote Early Babyhood Social and Emotional Development. Washington, DC: National Conference of State Legislatures and Zero to Three. (accessed on Dec 7, 2006)

Connell, J. P. 1990. "Context, Self, and Activity: A Motivational Analysis of Self-System Processes Across the Lifespan," in The Cocky in Transition: Infancy to Childhood. Edited by D. Cicchetti and Thousand. Beeghly. Chicago: The Academy of Chicago Printing.

Coplan, J. 1993. Early Language Milestone Scale: Examiner's Transmission (2d edition). Austin, TX: Pro-ed.

Davies, D. 2004. Kid Evolution: A Practitioner's Guide (Second edition). New York: Guilford Press.

DeCasper, A. J., and W. P. Fifer. 1980. "Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices," Science, Vol. 208, No. 6, 1174–76.

Denham, Due south. 1998. Emotional Evolution in Immature Children. New York: Guilford Press.

Denham, S., and R. Weissberg. 2003. "Social-Emotional Learning in Early on Babyhood: What We Know and Where to Go From Here," in A Pattern for the Promotion of Prosocial Behavior in Early Childhood. Edited by E. Chesebrough and others. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

Dickstein, S., and R. D. Parke. 1988. "Social Referencing in Infancy: A Glance at Fathers and Marriage," Child Evolution, Vol. 59, No. two, 506–11.

Dunn, J. 1983. "Sibling Relationships in Early Childhood," Kid Development, Vol. 54, No. four, 787–811.

Dunn, J. 1987. "The Beginnings of Moral Understanding: Development in the Second Yr," in The Emergence of Morality in Young Children. Edited past J. Kagan and S. Lamb. Chicago: Academy of Chicago Printing.

Dunn, J. 1988. The Beginnings of Social Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Printing.

Dunn, J. 1994. "Irresolute Minds and Irresolute Relationships," in Children'due south Early Understanding of Heed: Origins and Development. Edited past C. Lewis and P. Mitchell. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Dunn, J.; I. Bretherton; and P. Munn. 1987. "Conversations Near Feeling States Betwixt Mothers and Their Immature Children," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 23, No. one, 132–39.

Eisenberg, Due north. 2000. "Emotion, Regulation and Moral Development," Almanac Review of Psychology, Vol. 51, 665–97.

Eisenberg, North.; C. Champion; and Y. Ma. 2004. "Emotion-Related Regulation: An Emerging Construct," Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Vol. 50, No. three, 236–59.

Eisenberg, N., and others. 1993. "The Relations of Emotionality and Regulation to Preschoolers' Social Skills and Sociometric Status," Child Development, Vol. 64, 1418–38.

Eisenberg, N., and T. Spinrad. 2004. "Emotion-Related Regulation: Sharpening the Definition," Child Development, Vol. 75, No. 2, 334–39.

Return to Elevation

Fabes, R., and others. 2001. "Preschoolers' Spontaneous Emotion Vocabulary: Relations to Likability," Early Education & Development, Vol. 12, No. 1, 11–27.

Fernald, A. 1993. "Approval and Disapproval: Baby Responsiveness to Vocal Touch on in Familiar and Unfamiliar Languages," Child Development, Vol. 64, No. 3, 657–74.

Fogel, A. 2001. Infancy: Infant, Family, and Guild (Fourth edition). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.

Fox, N. A., and S. D. Calkins. 2000. "Multiple Mensurate Approaches to the Study of Babe Emotion," in Handbook of Emotions (2d edition). Edited by K. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Press.

Pull a fast one on, N. A., and R. J. Davidson. 1988. "Patterns of Brain Electrical Activeness During the Expression of Discrete Emotions in Ten-Calendar month-One-time Infants," Developmental Psychology, Vol.24, 230–36.

Frankenburg, West. K., and others. 1990. Denver 2 Screening Manual. Denver, CO: Denver Developmental Materials.

Fredrickson, B. 2000. "Cultivating Positive Emotions to Optimize Health and Well-Being," Prevention and Treatment, Vol. 3, No. one.

Fredrickson, B. 2003. "The Value of Positive Emotions," American Scientist, Vol. 91, 330–35.

Greenspan, S., and Northward. T. Greenspan. 1985. First Feelings: Milestones in the Emotional Development of Your Baby and Kid. New York: Penguin Books.

Gustafson, G. Due east.; J. A. Green; and M. J. West. 1979. "The Baby'southward Changing Role in Female parent-Infant Games: The Growth of Social Skills," Infant Behavior and Development, Vol. two, 301–viii.

Harris, P. L. 2000. "Understanding Emotion," in Handbook of Emotions (Second edition). Edited by M. Lewis and J. M. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Press.

Harris, P. Fifty., and others. 1989. "Young Children's Theory of Mind and Emotion," Cognition and Emotion, Vol. 3, No. iv, 379–400.

Hart, B., and T. R. Risley. 1999. The Social Globe of Children: Learning to Talk. Baltimore, Medico: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.

Hart, S., and others. 1998. "Jealousy Protests in Infants of Depressed Mothers," Infant Behavior and Development, Vol. 21, No. i, 137–48.

Hay, D. F.; J. Pederson; and A. Nash. 1982. "Dyadic Interaction in the Get-go Year of Life," in Peer Relationships and Social Skills in Childhood. Edited past One thousand. H. Rubin and H. Southward. Ross. New York: Springer-Verlag.

Hirshberg, Fifty. M., and M. Svejda. 1990. "When Infants Await to Their Parents: Infants' Social Referencing of Mothers Compared to Fathers," Child Development, Vol. 61, No. iv, 1175–86.

Hoffman, Yard. L. 1982. "Development of Prosocial Motivation: Empathy and Guilt," in The Development of Prosocial Behavior. Edited by N. Eisenberg. New York: Bookish Press.

Howes, C. 1983. "Patterns of Friendship," Child Development, Vol. 54, No. four, 1041–53.

Howes, C. 1987. "Social Competence with Peers in Immature Children: Developmental Sequences," Developmental Review, Vol. 7, 252–72.

Howes, C. 1988. "Peer Interaction of Young Children," Monographs of the Society for Enquiry in Child Development, Vol. 53, No. ane.

Howes, C., and C. C. Matheson. 1992. "Sequences in the Evolution of Competent Play with Peers: Social and Social Pretend Play," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, No. 5, 961–74.

Johnson, Thousand., and others. 1991. "Newborns' Preferential Tracking of Face up-Similar Stimuli and Its Subsequent Reject," Cognition, Vol. twoscore, Nos. i-ii, ane–xix.

Johnstone, T., and K. R., Scherer. 2000. "Vocal Communication of Emotion," in Handbook of Emotions (Second edition). Edited by M. Lewis and J. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Press.

Kaler, S. R., and C. B. Kopp. 1990. "Compliance and Comprehension in Very Young Toddlers," Kid Development, Vol. 61, No. 6, 1997–2003.

Kaye, K., and A. Fogel. 1980. "The Temporal Structure of Face-to-Face up Communication Between Mothers and Infants," Developmental Psychology, Vol. xvi, 454–64.

Kontos, S., and A. Wilcox-Herzog. January, 1997. "Research in Review: Teacher's Interactions with Children: Why Are They So Of import?" Immature Children, Vol. 52, No. ii, iv–12.

Kopp, C. 1989. "Regulation of Distress and Negative Emotions: A Developmental View," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 25, No. iii, 343–54.

Render to Top

Kravitz, H.; D. Goldenberg; and A. Neyhus. 1978. "Tactual Exploration past Normal Infants," Developmental Medicine and Kid-Neurology, Vol. 20, No. six, 720–26.

Lagattuta, 1000. H., and R. A. Thompson. 2007. "The Evolution of Self-Conscious Emotions: Cognitive Processes and Social Influences," in The Self-Conscious Emotions: Theory and Research. Edited by J. L. Tracy, R. West. Robins, and J. P. Tangney. New York: Guilford Printing.

Lally, J. R., and others. 1995. Caring for Infants and Toddlers in Groups: Developmentally Appropriate Practice. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press.

Lamb, Thou. E.; Chiliad. H. Bornstein; and D. M. Teti. 2002. Development in Infancy: An Introduction (Fourth edition). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Lerner, C., and L. A. Ciervo. 2003. Salubrious Minds: Nurturing Children's Development from 0 to 36 Months. Washington, DC: Naught to Three Press and American Academy of Pediatrics.

Lerner, C., and A. L. Dombro. 2000. Learning and Growing Together: Understanding and Supporting Your Child'southward Development. Washington, DC: Zero to Three Press.

Levenson, R., and A. Ruef. 1992. "Empathy: A Physiological Substrate," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 63, No. ii, 234–46.

Levine, L. E. 1983. "Mine: Self-Definition in 2-Year-Quondam Boys," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 19, 544–49.

Lewis, M. 2000a. "The Emergence of Man Emotions," in Handbook of Emotions (Second edition). Edited past M. Lewis and J. Grand. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Press.

Lewis, K. 2000b. "Self-Witting Emotions: Embarrassment, Pride, Shame, and Guilt," in Handbook of Emotions (Second edition). Edited past M. Lewis and J. K. Haviland-Jones. New York: Guilford Press.

Lewis, M., and J. Brooks-Gunn. 1979. Social Cognition and the Acquisition of Self. New York: Plenum Press.

Lewis, K., and others. 1989. "Cocky Evolution and Self-Conscious Emotions," Kid Evolution, Vol. 60, No. ane, 146–56.

Lieberman, A. F. 1993. The Emotional Life of the Toddler. New York: Free Printing.

Marvin, R., and P. Britner. 1999. "Normative Evolution: The Ontogeny of Attachment," in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Enquiry, and Clinical Applications. Edited past J. Cassidy and P. Shaver. New York: Guilford Press.

McCarty, M. E.; R. K. Clifton; and R. R. Collard. 1999. "Problem Solving in Infancy: The Emergence of an Action Program," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 35, No. four, 1091–1101.

Meisels, S. J., and others. 2003. The Ounce Scale: Standards for the Developmental Profiles (Nascence–42 Months). New York: Pearson Early Learning.

Melson, Chiliad., and A. Cohen. 1981. "Contextual Influences on Children's Activity: Sexual practice Differences in Effects of Peer Presence and Interpersonal Allure," Genetic Psychology Monographs, Vol. 103, 243–60.

Messinger, D., and A. Fogel. 2007. "The Interactive Evolution of Social Grin," in Advances in Child Development and Behavior (Vol. 35). Edited by R. V. Kail. Burlington, MA: Elsevier.

Mesquita, B., and Due north. Frijda. 1992. "Cultural Variations in Emotions: A Review," Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 112, No. 2, 179–204.

Mueller, Due east., and T. Lucas. 1975. "A Developmental Analysis of Peer Interaction Among Toddlers," in Friendship and Peer Relations. Edited by One thousand. Lewis and L. Rosenblum. New York: Wiley.

National Research Council and Establish of Medicine. 2000. From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. Committee on Integrating the Science of Early Childhood Evolution. Edited by J. P. Shonkoff and D. A. Phillips. Lath on Children, Youth and Families, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Instruction. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.

National Scientific Council on the Developing Child. Winter, 2004. "Children's Emotional Evolution Is Congenital into the Architecture of Their Brains" Working Newspaper No. 2 External link opens in new window or tab.. (accessed on December five, 2006)

Return to Peak

 Panksepp, J. 2001. The Long-Term Psychobiological Consequences of Infant Emotions: Prescriptions for the Twenty-Commencement Century," Babe Mental Wellness Journal, Vol. 22, No. one–2, 132–73.

Parks, S. 2004. Inside Help: Hawaii Early Learning Profile Administration and Reference Manual. Palo Alto, CA: VORT Corporation.

Parritz, R. H. 1996. "A Descriptive Analysis of Toddler Coping in Challenging Circumstances," Infant Beliefs and Development, Vol. nineteen, 171–80.

Pruett, K. D. 1999. Me, Myself and I: How Children Build Their Sense of Cocky (eighteen to 36 Months). New York: Goddard Press.

Quann, V., and C. Wien. 2006. "The Visible Empathy of Infants and Toddlers," Immature Children, Vol. 61, No. 4, 22–29.

Raikes, H. 1996. "A Secure Base for Babies: Applying Attachment Concepts to the Baby Care Setting," Young Children, Vol. 51, No. v, 59–67.

Raver, C. 2002. "Emotions Matter: Making the Instance for the Office of Young Children'due south Emotional Evolution for Early School Readiness," SRCD Social Policy Report, Vol. 16, No. 3.

Ross, H. S., and B. D. Goldman. 1977. "Establishing New Social Relations in Infancy," in Attachment Beliefs. Edited by T. Alloway, P. Pliner, and L. Krames. New York: Plenum Printing.

Rothbart, Chiliad. Thousand.; H. Ziaie; and C. Thousand. O'Boyle. 1992. "Cocky-Regulation and Emotion in Infancy," in Emotion and Its Regulation in Early on Development (No. 55). Edited past N. Eisenberg and R. Fabes. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass/Pfeiffer.

Ryalls, B.; R. Gul; and K. Ryalls. 2000. "Infant Faux of Peer and Adult Models: Bear witness for a Peer Model Advantage," Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Vol. 46, No.1, 188–202.

Saarni, C., and others. 2006. "Emotional Evolution: Action, Communication, and Understanding," in Handbook of Child Psychology (Sixth edition), Vol. 3, Social, Emotional, and Personality Evolution. Edited by N. Eisenberg. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.

Schaffer, H. R., and P. East. Emerson. 1964. "The Development of Social Attachments in Infancy," Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 29, No. 3.

Segal, Yard. 2004. "The Roots and Fruits of Pretending," in Children'southward Play: The Roots of Reading. Edited by E. F. Zigler, D. 1000. Vocaliser, and Due south. J. Bishop-Josef. Washington, DC: Zilch to 3 Press.

Shonkoff, J. P. 2004. Science, Policy and the Developing Child: Endmost the Gap Between What Nosotros Know and What Nosotros Practise. Washington, DC: Ounce of Prevention Fund. http://world wide web.ounceofprevention.org/downloads/publications/shonkoffweb.pdf (accessed on December 7, 2006) Note, the preceding Spider web address is no longer valid.

Siegel, D. J. 1999. The Developing Mind: How Relationships and the Encephalon Collaborate to Shape Who We Are. New York: Guilford Printing.

Squires, J.; D. Bricker; and E. Twombly. 2002. The Ages & Stages Questionnaires: Social-Emotional. A Parent-Completed Child-Monitoring Arrangement for Social-Emotional Behaviors ASQ:SE. User'southward Guide. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.

Sroufe, L. A. 1979. "Socioemotional Development," in Handbook of Baby Evolution. Edited by J. Osofsky. New York: Wiley.

Stern, D. N. 1985. The Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Developmental Psychology. New York: Basic Books.

Sternberg, C. R.; J. J. Campos; and R. Due north. Emde. 1983. "The Facial Expression of Anger in Seven-Month-Quondam Infants," Child Development, Vol. 54, 178–84.

Stipek, D. J.; J. H. Gralinski; and C. B. Kopp. 1990. "Cocky-Concept Evolution in the Toddler Years," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 26, No. vi, 972–77.

Taumoepeau, M., and T. Ruffman. 2008. "Stepping Stones to Others' Minds: Maternal Talk Relates to Child Mental State Language and Emotion Agreement at xv, 24, and 33 Months," Kid Development, Vol. 79, No. two, 284–302.

Teti, D. One thousand. 1999. "Conceptualizations of Disorganization in the Preschool Years: An Integration," in Zipper Disorganization. Edited by J. Solomon and C. George. New York: Guilford Press.

Render to Pinnacle

Thelen, E., and A. Fogel. 1989. "Toward an Action-Based Theory of Babe Development," in Action in Social Context: Perspectives on Early Development. Edited by J. Lockman and N. Hazen. New York: Plenum Press.

Thompson, R. A. 1987. "Empathy and Emotional Understanding: The Early Development of Empathy," in Empathy and Its Development. Edited past Due north. Eisenberg and J. Strayer. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Thompson, R. A. 2006. "The Development of the Person: Social Understanding, Relationships, Cocky, Censor," in Handbook of Kid Psychology (6th edition), Book 3: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development. Edited by N. Eisenberg. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley and Sons.

Thompson, R. A., and R. Goodvin. 2005. "The Private Child: Temperament, Emotion, Self and Personality," in Developmental Scientific discipline: An Avant-garde Textbook (Fifth edition). Edited past M. H. Bornstein and Grand. Due east. Lamb. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Tronick, Due east. Z. 1989. "Emotions and Emotional Communication in Infants," American Psychologist, Vol. 44, No. 2, 112–19.

Tsai, J.; B. Knutson; and H. Fung. 2006. "Cultural Variation in Affect Valuation," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 90, No. 2, 288–307.

Tsai, J.; R. Levenson; and K. McCoy. 2006. "Cultural and Temperamental Variation in Emotional Response," Emotion, Vol. 6, No. 3, 484–97.

Tsai, J., and others. 2007. "Learning What Feelings to Desire: Socialization of Ideal Impact Through Children'southward Storybooks," Personality and Social Psychology Message, Vol. 33, No. 1, 17–thirty.

Weinberg, M. Yard., and others. 1999. "Gender Differences in Emotional Expressivity and Self-Regulation During Early Infancy," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 35, No. 1, 175–88.

Wellman, H. M., and Thousand. H. Lagattuta. 2000. "Developing Understandings of Mind," in Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cerebral Neuroscience. Edited by Due south. Baron-Cohen, T. Tager-Flusberg, and D. J. Cohen. New York: Oxford University Press.

Wingert, P., and K. Brant. 2005. "Reading Your Baby's Heed," Newsweek, Aug. 15, 2005, 32–39.

Yuill, N. 1984. "Young Children'south Coordination of Motive and Outcome in Judgments of Satisfaction and Morality," British Journal of Developmental Psychology, Vol. 2, 73–81.

Zahn-Waxler, C., and Yard. Radke-Yarrow. 1982. "The Development of Altruism: Culling Research Strategies," in The Development of Prosocial Behavior. Edited by N. Eisenberg. New York: Academic Printing.

Zahn-Waxler, C., and M. Radke-Yarrow. 1990. "The Origins of Empathetic Concern," Motivation and Emotion, Vol. xiv, 107–thirty.

Zahn-Waxler, C., and others. 1992. "Development of Concern for Others," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, No. one, 126–36.

Zahn-Waxler, C.; J. Robinson; and R. Emde. 1992. "Development of Empathy in Twins," Developmental Psychology, Vol. 28, No. six, 1038–47.

Zip to Three. 2004. Infant and Early Babyhood Mental Wellness: Promoting Healthy Social and Emotional Development. Fact Sheet. May xviii, 2004. Washington, DC: Zippo to Three. (accessed on Dec vii, 2006)

Return to Top

Return to Contents

Final Reviewed: Thursday, June 17, 2021

mcvicarsolike1953.blogspot.com

Source: https://www.cde.ca.gov/sp/cd/re/itf09socemodev.asp

0 Response to "Child Care Center Curriculum Plan Involving Families to Enhance Social Emotional Behaviors"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel